Traits such as sociability and aggressiveness are thought to be _________.

  • Journal List
  • HHS Author Manuscripts
  • PMC1188235

J Dev Behav Pediatr. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2005 Aug 22.

Published in final edited form as:

PMCID: PMC1188235

NIHMSID: NIHMS2462

Abstract

Most temperament theories presume a biological basis to those behavioral tendencies thought to be temperamental in origin. Behavioral genetic methods can be used to test this assumption. Twin and adoption studies suggest that individual differences in infant and child temperament are genetically influenced. However, behavioral genetics has much more to offer to the study of temperament than simple heritability estimates. The present paper describes some recent findings from behavioral genetics research in temperament that go well beyond the basic nature-nurture question. These findings include the importance of nonshared environmental influences on temperament, genetic continuity and environmental change during development, links between temperament and behavior problems, and harnessing the power of molecular genetics to identify specific genes responsible for genetic influence on early temperament.

Keywords: genetic, shared environment, nonshared environment, heritability, temperament

Although there are many different theories of child temperament, most agree that temperament refers to stable, early appearing individual differences in behavioral tendencies that have a constitutional basis1. Soon after birth, children show a great deal of variation in those behavioral dimensions considered to be temperamental (e.g., emotionality, activity level, attention/persistence, sociability, reactivity, etc.). For example, some children cry easily and intensely whereas others are more easy going; some are highly active and always on the go where others are more sedentary; some attend and persist in tasks for long periods of time where others’ attention wanders quickly. It is these individual differences and the variations in between that are of interest to behavioral geneticists. That is, we are interested in understanding why children differ in their temperaments. Temperament theories suggest that such individual differences have a biological or constitutional foundation. This is an empirical question that can be answered using behavioral genetic methods.

After a brief review of behavioral genetic methodology, this paper will examine research exploring the etiology of individual differences in infant and child temperament. Behavioral genetics research, however, has much more to offer the study of temperament than estimates of genetic influence. To that end, this paper will also examine the nature of environmental influences on temperament; the etiology of continuity and change in temperament; links between temperament and behavior problems; and the merging of behavioral and molecular genetic techniques to identify specific genes that affect temperament.

Quantitative Behavioral Genetics Methods

A goal of behavioral genetic research is to estimate the extent to which genetic and environmental factors contribute to behavioral variability in the population under study. This involves decomposing the observed (i.e., phenotypic) variance of a trait into genetic and environmental variance components. Heritability (h2), the genetic effect size, is the proportion of phenotypic variance that can be attributed to genetic factors. The remaining variance is attributed to environmental factors which comprise all nonheritable influences including prenatal factors. Environmental variance can be further decomposed into shared and nonshared environmental influences. Shared environmental variance (c2) is familial resemblance that is not explained by genetic variance. Thus, shared environmental variance includes those environmental influences that are shared by family members and act to enhance familial similarity. Nonshared environmental variance (e2) is a residual variance that includes measurement error and environmental influences that are unique to each individual. These unique environmental influences operate to make members of the same family different from one another. Possible sources of nonshared environmental variance include differential parental treatment; extrafamilial relationships with friends, peers and teachers; and nonsystematic factors such as accidents or illness2.

The two designs most frequently used to disentangle genetic and environmental sources of variance in infant and child temperament are the twin design and the adoptive/nonadoptive sibling design. The twin method involves comparing genetically identical (MZ) twins with fraternal (DZ) twins who share approximately 50% of their segregating genes. Genetic influences are implied when co-twin similarity covaries with the degree of genetic relatedness. Thus, according to the genetic hypothesis, MZ twins should be approximately twice as similar as DZ twins. DZ twin resemblance that exceeds that predicted by the genetic hypothesis (i.e., resemblance greater than one-half the MZ twin resemblance) suggests the presence of shared environmental influences. Because MZ twins share all of their genes, differences within pairs of identical twins can only be due to environmental influences that are unique to each individual (i.e., MZ twin differences indicate nonshared environmental influences). The adoptive/nonadoptive sibling design shares a similar logic, but compares the similarity of adoptive and nonadoptive sibling pairs. Genetic influences are implied when nonadoptive siblings who share approximately 50% of their segregating genes are more similar than adoptive siblings who are not genetically related. Shared environmental influences are suggested when genetically unrelated adoptive siblings resemble each other. (See Plomin, DeFries, McClearn & McGuffin3 for more information about behavioral genetics methods).

Behavioral Genetic Studies of Temperament

Although Thomas and Chess’ theoretical perspective based on data from the New York Longitudinal Study (NYLS)4 is most common to the pediatric literature, no single theory dominates behavioral genetic studies of temperament. As indicated above, there are many different theories of temperament. These approaches differ in those behavioral dimensions that are considered to fall under the rubric of temperament, however, some dimensions, such as activity level and emotionality, appear in nearly all theories of temperament1. (See Goldsmith et al. 19871 for a discussion of theoretical orientations to temperament).

Two measures that are frequently employed in behavioral genetic studies of temperament are the Colorado Child Temperament Inventory (CCTI)5 and the Infant Behavior Record (IBR)6. The CCTI is a questionnaire measure that is based on factor analyses of items designed to assess both Buss and Plomin’s7 EAS (emotionality, activity, sociability) temperaments and the nine NYLS temperaments. The parent and teacher versions of the CCTI assess Emotionality (negative emotionality including distress, fear, and anger), Activity (the tempo, energy, and vigor with which the child behaves), Sociability (a preference to be with other people), Shyness (wariness with strangers), and Attention/Persistence (the tendency to attend and persist when working on tasks). The IBR is often used to provide an observational measure of infant temperament. This rating scale is typically completed by an examiner following administration of the Bayley Scales of Infant Development6. The IBR consists of items evaluating broad dimensions of infant behavior, including interpersonal, affective, motivational, and sensory behavioral domains. Factor analyses of the IBR items yields three factors related to dimensions found in most systems of temperament: Activity (energy and body movement), Task Orientation (attention span and persistence), and Affect-Extraversion (positive emotionality and sociability)8, 9. The temperament dimensions assessed by these two measures are well-studied in behavioral genetics research, however, as will be seen below, a variety of other dimensions have also been explored.

Genetic Influences on Child Temperament

Twin studies using parent ratings, the most frequently employed measure of temperament in infancy and childhood, provide strong evidence of genetic influences on temperament. Such studies consistently find that MZ cotwins are more similar than DZ cotwins across a wide variety of temperament dimensions including emotionality, activity, shyness, sociability, attention/persistence, approach, adaptability, distress, positive affect and negative affect10–13. Although estimates of heritability tend to differ from sample to sample, they generally fall within the range of .20 to .60, suggesting that genetic differences among individuals account for approximately 20% to 60% of the variability of temperament within a population. With few exceptions (e.g., soothability and rhythmicity, which show little genetic influence), there is no consistent pattern of differential heritability across dimensions.

Given that temperament is assumed to be biologically based1, it is not surprising to find that parent-rated temperament is genetically influenced. What is surprising, however, is the unusual pattern of twin resemblances (as indexed by twin correlations) that frequently emerge when temperament is assessed by parent ratings. MZ correlations for parent-rated temperament dimensions are typically moderate; whereas DZ correlations are much lower than one half the MZ correlations as would be predicted from the simple genetic model. In fact, the DZ correlations are often near zero or even negative13–15. The pattern of very low DZ correlations that emerges with parent ratings of temperament is significant because it implies that DZ twins are perceived as no more similar as two randomly-paired children, and in some instances are regarded as having opposing temperaments.

Also puzzling is the finding that although twin studies consistently yield evidence of a genetic influence on parent-rated temperament, adoption studies suggest little or no genetic influences on children’s temperament as rated by their parents. For example, in the Colorado Adoption Project16, 17 (CAP), neither genetically related nonadoptive siblings nor genetically unrelated adoptive siblings displayed any resemblance on parent reports of temperament—a finding that replicated across early childhood, middle childhood, and early adolescence18–20. Similarly, a combined twin and stepfamily study found significant genetic influences on each of the four EAS7 dimensions; however, heritability estimates were greater for twins than nontwins21.

These puzzling outcomes do not arise when temperament is assessed via more objective measures such as tester and observer ratings or mechanical measures9, 15, 22–27. Although less frequent than parent-rating studies of temperament, there are a handful of twin studies that have employed observational/behavioral measures of activity level, behavioral inhibition, shyness, fearfulness, affect/extraversion, and task orientation. As with parent ratings, these measures yield evidence of significant genetic influence (and a similar range of heritabilities, i.e., also from .20 to .60), but the problem of low DZ correlations does not emerge. For example, in the MacArthur Longitudinal Twin Study28 (MALTS) observational measures of affect/extraversion, activity, task orientation and shyness were obtained for over 200 twin pairs at 14, 20, 24, and 36 months of age. Averaging across age, the DZ correlations for all dimensions were positive and significantly different from zero (ranging from .19 to .63); and consistent with genetic expectations, the DZ twin correlations were approximately half that of the MZ twins29. Interestingly, when parents rated the same twins on similar dimensions the DZ twin correlations were negative (ranging from −.06 to −.33). Thus, parents reported their DZ twins has having opposing temperaments, whereas when each twin was rated by a different observer, the twins displayed substantial similarity. Although it is possible that contextual differences explain the different outcomes between parent and observer ratings of temperament, this seems unlikely given that similar results have emerged from twin studies of activity level where parent ratings and mechanical measures of activity are based on the same 48-hour period26,30.

Similarly, evidence of genetic influences on temperament emerges from adoption studies when more objective measures of temperament are employed. In the CAP, teacher and tester ratings of temperament yielded correlations for nonadoptive siblings that were higher than when assessed via parent ratings; and consistent with a genetic hypothesis, nonadoptive siblings showed some resemblance in their temperaments, but adoptive siblings did not18, 31. Moreover, adoption and twin studies of objectively-assessed temperament yield similar estimates of heritability32.

The fact that the problems of very low DZ resemblance and higher heritability estimates in twin studies as compared to adoption studies occur only for parent-rating measures of temperament suggests that parent ratings are prone to contrast effects12, 29, 33. Contrast effects refer to rater biases that exaggerate the differences between cotwins or nonadoptive siblings. Twin research suggests that some parent rating measures may be more subject to contrast effects than others. For example, very low DZ correlations tend to be particularly evident when parents are required to make global judgments (e.g., “always on the go”) as opposed to specific judgments (e.g. “splashes when in the tub”) about their children’s behavior34–36; however, even specific measures can yield low DZ correlations11. There are also hints that the tendency to contrast siblings differs across temperament dimensions. Activity level, attention/persistence and shyness are dimensions that frequently show contrast effects; whereas parent ratings of more affective behavioral dimensions such as approach, fear, pleasure, smiling and laughter do not seem to show a pattern of low DZ or nonadoptive sibling similarity11, 37.

Because contrast effects can overestimate heritability in twin designs and underestimate heritability in adoption designs, parent-rating measures that show a pattern of very low DZ or sibling correlations may be inadequate for obtaining precise estimates of heritability unless such biases are incorporated into the statistical analyses (e.g., sibling interaction models). Nonetheless, the greater similarity of MZ than DZ twins is consistent with the general hypothesis of a genetic influence. Moreover, conclusions regarding moderate genetic influences on individual differences in parent-rated temperament are buttressed by similar findings with more objective temperament measures. Thus, there can be little doubt that temperament is genetically influenced.

Environmental Influences on Child Temperament

The finding of moderate genetic influences on child temperament does not negate the importance of the environment. As indicated above, genetic factors account for between 20% and 60% of the phenotypic variance in personality, which means that the remaining 80% to 40% of the variance is attributed to environmental factors. Clearly, the environment is very important to temperament. However, behavioral genetics research suggests that the types of environments traditionally assumed to influence child behavior may not operate the way we think they do. Twin and adoption studies consistently find that shared family environment accounts for only a small portion of variance in most temperament dimensions10, 12, 18, 24, 25, 32, 38. This is demonstrated in a study of infant temperament that found correlations for tester-rated temperament to be about .00 for genetically unrelated adoptive siblings—which provides a direct test of shared family environment—and .20 for genetically related nonadoptive siblings32. In other words, growing up in the same family does not make family members resemble each other in temperaments. Family members are similar in temperaments primarily because of shared DNA.

If shared family environments do not substantially influence temperament, then what does? The answer lies within, not between, families. The environmental influences that are important to temperament are those factors that are not shared by members of the same family—that is, environmental influences that are unique to family members (i.e., nonshared environmental influences). The finding of significant and substantial nonshared environmental influences on temperament provides an important focus for researchers interested in environmental effects on temperament. Most research exploring environmental influences on temperament have considered between-family effects such as parenting style and family functioning (e.g., Eriksson & Pehrsson39, Leve, Scaramella & Fagot40). Behavioral genetics research suggests that instead of examining environmental factors that differ across families, it will be more profitable to focus on environmental factors that differ within families (e.g., differential parenting). Researchers need to consider why individuals within the same family differ so much with regard to temperament. This will involve studying more than one individual per family and exploring the association of experiential differences within a family with differences in temperament.

There are some exceptions to the general finding of no shared environmental influences on temperament. Several studies have found shared environmental influences on both parent and observer ratings of positive affect and related behaviors (e.g., smiling, interest in others) during infancy and early childhood11, 41–45. Maternal personality and attachment security have been suggested as possible sources of shared environmental variance on positive affect44. In addition, although not well-studied, rhythmicity and soothability have shown no genetic influence but substantial shared environmental influences each in at least one study10, 44. These findings need to be replicated, but it is possible that, to some extent, the shared environmental influences on these two dimensions reflect parenting behaviors (e.g., scheduling of activities, methods of soothing). Finally, observational measures of shyness and mechanical measures of activity show some shared environmental variance, whereas parent ratings of the same behaviors do not15, 22, 26, 27, 46. Again, these findings need to be replicated but they highlight the need for more observational research to assess the possible role of shared environment in personality development.

The Etiology of Continuity and Change in Temperament

Despite the abundance of evidence indicating that individual differences in temperament are genetically influenced, little is known about the role that genetic factors play in the development of temperament. The failure to consider genetic contributions to developmental change in temperament has likely resulted from the mistaken view that genetic factors are immutable and thus, contribute only to behavioral stability. However, genes are dynamic in nature, changing in the quantity and quality of their effects across time and therefore, can be sources of change as well as continuity in behavioral development47. Behavioral genetics approaches developmental change in two ways. The first explores differential heritability across ages. This issue is important because the investigation of the etiology of individual differences over age may serve to identify points of causal transition. The second examines the role of genetic influences on continuity and change during development, and thus addresses the process by which developmental change takes place15.

Differential heritability refers to developmental changes in the relative contribution of genetic influences to individual differences in temperament across age. That is, does the heritability of temperament change across age? Intuitively, one might posit that as a child matures and becomes more interactive with increasingly diverse environments, the role of genetic factors on temperament might wane. Empirical research does not support this notion. Many studies find no apparent change in heritability across age25, 27, 48. Moreover, when developmental changes in genetic influences on temperament are apparent, it is in the direction of increased genetic variance10, 13, 32. For example, in the Louisville Twin Study there was no evidence of a genetic effect on temperament during the neonatal period49, but in infancy and early childhood temperament was moderately heritable23, 50.

To the developmentalist, the measurement of within-person change is more interesting than cross-sectional age differences because the measurement of change is more informative about underlying processes51. For the same reason, genetic influences on continuity and change are more interesting than the presence or absence of genetic influences at a single age. The question of differential heritability is essentially cross-sectional and does not address mechanisms of change. That is, for any temperament dimension, estimates of heritability may differ across age even though the same genes operate at each age. Similarly, the heritability of a temperament dimension may be similar across two ages, but the genes that operate at one age might differ from those that operate at the other. By using behavioral genetic methods within a longitudinal design we are able to determine the extent to which developmental change and continuity are due to genetic factors.

Longitudinal behavioral genetic studies of early temperament development are rare. One exception is the Louisville Twin Study which found that MZ twins demonstrated greater resemblance than DZ twins on age-to-age change profiles for observational measures of activity, affect/extraversion, and task orientation across 6 to 18 months and 12 to 24 months, emotional tone across 18 to 24 months, and surgency (related to extraversion) across 36 to 48 months of age23, 52, 53. In other words, MZ twins were more similar than DZ twins in their patterns of change for these temperament dimensions. These results suggest that changes in temperament across infancy and early childhood are, in part, regulated by genetic influences.

Although the analysis of change profiles provides important information about genetic influences on developmental change, they do not reveal the extent to which the genetic effects that influence a trait are consistent from one age to another47. This can be addressed by assessing genetic contributions to phenotypic continuity across age. Genetic influences on phenotypic continuity imply that there is some overlap between the genetic factors that affect a trait across age54. Longitudinal model-fitting analyses of genetic contributions to phenotypic continuity permit the estimation of the extent to which genetic effects on a trait at one age overlap with genetic effects at another age and whether new genetic influences on the trait emerge across time. To date, most of what we know about the etiology of continuity and change in temperament in infancy and early childhood comes from the MALTS.

Observer ratings of activity, affect/extraversion, task orientation, shyness and behavioral inhibition at 14, 20, 24 and 36 months of age in over 200 twin pairs participating in MALTS yielded modest age-to-age stability correlations (i.e., ranging from .00 to .39), indicating that phenotypically, there is substantial change in these temperament dimensions over the second year of life25, 55. Despite this, for most dimensions, genetic influences appear to be mediating whatever continuity exists. The age-to-age stability correlations for activity, affect/extraversion, and behavioral inhibition were almost entirely due to genetic factors. Shyness was an exception in that both genetic and shared environmental influences contributed to stability. Developmental change in observed temperament was primarily due to nonshared environmental influences; however, for activity, affect/extraversion, task orientation, and behavioral inhibition there was evidence of new genetic effects at different ages. Similarly, for shyness, new shared environmental influences emerged across age. Thus, with the exception of shyness, genetic factors appear to contribute to both continuity and change in temperament across age. A caveat is that in some instances, genetic change coincided with subtle changes in the item content of the measures used to assess temperament; consequently, developmental change was confounded with measurement differences. Nonetheless, these findings of genetic change would be consistent with earlier MALTS analyses that found significant genetic change on activity and sociability when assessed with the same parent-rating measure at 14 to 20 months of age15.

Overall, the MALTS results are consistent with longitudinal twin studies of adult personality that find that personality change is largely due to nonshared environmental influences, and the stability of personality is due to genetic factors56. This was unexpected because, in contrast to adulthood, infancy and early childhood is a period of rapid change and growth. That developmental change is primarily due to early nonshared environmental influences is particularly interesting. This means that changes in temperament are likely due to differences within the family environment, such as differential treatment, experiences or accidents. Identifying specific nonshared environmental factors that influence developmental change is an important goal for future temperament research.

The significance of temperament lies, in part, in the assumption that early temperament shapes personality development and influences developmental outcomes57. Child temperament has been seen as a possible precursor to behavior problems58, 59. Indeed, the link between temperament in early childhood and later behavioral problems has been well documented. For example, dimensions associated with difficult temperament (e.g., emotionality, adaptability, approach/withdrawal, impulsivity) are predictive of both internalizing and externalizing problems60–63, activity level predicts externalizing problems64–66, and shyness and fearfulness predict internalizing problems67, 68. Importantly, these associations do not appear to be an artifact due to similarity of items on measures used to assess both temperament and problem behavior62, 69.

Although there is ample evidence of an association between temperament and behavior problems, the question of how the association arises has received little empirical attention. That is, what are the mechanisms that link temperament and behavior problems? Behavioral genetic research can address this question by exploring the extent to which temperament and behavior problems are influenced by common genetic and environmental factors. There have been many twin and adoption studies that indicate substantial genetic influence (i.e., heritiabilities in the range of .30 to .50) for externalizing, internalizing, and total behavior problems70–74. Thus, given that both temperament and behavior problems are genetically influenced it is possible that they are associated because of common genetic factors.

The few studies that have examined sources of covariance between temperament and behavior problems suggest that the association is primarily due to common genetic influences. For example, in MALTS, emotionality at 14, 20, 24, and 36 months predicted internalizing, externalizing and total behavior problems at age 4 years, and shyness predicted internalizing across the same age intervals68. There was considerable overlap in genetic effects on temperament and behavior problems. Moreover, it was these overlapping genetic effects that fully explained the phenotypic correlations between temperament and behavior problems. Similarly, in the CAP, emotionality in later childhood also predicted internalizing, externalizing and total behavior problems75. Although there was some genetic variance unique to emotionality and behavior problems, there was also substantial genetic overlap between the two, and the phenotypic associations between teacher ratings of both and between teachers’ ratings of emotionality and mothers’ reports of problem behaviors were primarily due to genetic factors.

Other research has looked at more specific problem behaviors, but a similar pattern emerges. In a sample of twins spanning middle childhood to adolescence, emotionality significantly predicted anxious/depressed, attention, delinquent and aggressive behavior problems assessed two years later76. The phenotypic correlations between emotionality and aggression and attention problems were due to common genetic factors. There were no significant shared environmental influences on any of the observed associations. Likewise, a study of fear and anxiety symptoms in early childhood found that despite only modest overlap in the genetic influences on fear and general anxiety, the phenotypic correlation between the two was almost entirely due to genetic factors67. Interestingly, however, fear and separation anxiety were genetically distinct and correlated because of common shared and nonshared environmental influences. Thus, links between fear and general anxiety and fear and separation anxiety arise for different reasons.

Research examining genetic and environmental links between temperament and later behavior problems has significance for both genetics researchers and clinicians. A finding of substantial overlap in genetic influences on the two domains would provide empirical support for the possibility that certain behavior problems may be the extreme manifestation of specific temperament dimensions. On the other hand, in those cases where temperament and behavior problems are genetically distinct (e.g., fear and separation anxiety), it is important to uncover the specific environmental factors that link temperament to later problem behavior. Both outcomes have implications for intervention. Moreover, in both cases, molecular genetics findings on one domain may inform about genes operating (or not operating) on the other.

Finding Genes Associated With Temperament

Twin and adoption analyses provide strong evidence that genes influence individual differences in temperament, continuity and change in temperament and, at least in part, mediate the link between temperament and behavior problems. However, the genetic effects in these analyses are anonymous. That is, quantitative genetic designs indicate the magnitude of genetic influence, but do not identify specific genes responsible for heritability. One of the most exciting new directions for research on personality and temperament comes from recent advances in molecular genetic techniques that now make it possible to identify genes associated with complex phenotypes.

Complex behavioral traits are unlikely to result from the action of major genes segregating in a simple Mendelian fashion, but are expected to result from the action or co-action of a few (oligo) or many (poly) genes77. Behavioral dimensions such as those included under the rubric of temperament are typically distributed continuously in general populations, show substantial environmental influence as well as genetic influence, and are likely to be influenced by many genes, each of varying effect78. Genes of small and varying effect size, which contribute to quantitative traits, are referred to as quantitative trait loci (QTLs). The challenge for contemporary molecular geneticists is to use the power of modern molecular techniques and new high-density genetic maps to identify QTLs for complex traits like temperament dimensions that involve multiple-genetic and non-genetic factors. The goal of applying molecular genetic techniques to the study of behavior is not aimed at identifying the gene for a particular behavioral dimension, rather it is to identify many genes that each make a small contribution to variability in a particular trait77.

Although the merging of molecular genetics and quantitative genetics is still in its infancy, researchers are beginning to identify genes associated with childhood behavioral dimensions and disorders (reviewed in Asherson & Curran79). Perhaps most robust is the finding that the dopamine D4 receptor gene (DRD4) has been associated with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), accounting for approximately 2% of genetic variance (see Mill et al.80 for a review). This is of interest to temperament researchers because activity level, as a temperament dimension, has been associated with diagnoses of ADHD and hyperactive problem behaviors in both clinical and nonclinical samples81–88, suggesting that hyperactivity may represent the extreme upper-end of the normal distribution of the temperament dimension of activity level. Consequently, genes associated with ADHD may also be associated with the temperament dimension of activity level.

Associations between genes and personality traits have also emerged, but are less well replicated (see Reif & Lesch89). For example, a 48-base pair repeat in exon 3 of DRD4 has been associated with novelty seeking90, 91 and a serotonin transporter promoter polymorphism (5-HTTLPR) has been associated with neuroticism/anxiety92; but these associations are questionable due to failures to replicate93–95. More specific to child temperament, both the DRD4 and 5-HTTLPR polymorphisms have been associated with motor organization and negative emotionality (i.e., the tendency to fear, anger, and distress) in early infancy96, 97. DRD4 was also associated with observed attention, interest, anger, and activity level; and 5-HTTLPR associated with stranger fearfulness and positive emotionality at 1 year of age in the same sample98, 99. Other research has found an interactive effect between DRD4 and 5-HTTLPR and stranger fearfulness in 1-year-old infants100. A recent attempt to explore the association of the DRD4 and 5-HTTLPR polymorphisms with shyness and aggression in toddlers has produced mixed results. Although neither polymorphism was associated with observed shyness, DRD4 was associated with mother-reported, but not observed, aggression101. Similarly, DRD4 was associated with infant adaptability at 1 month, but not 5 months of age102. These early temperament results should be viewed with caution until they have been replicated in a variety of samples. Nonetheless, they hint at the potential for the application of molecular genetic approaches to the study of temperament. This is a particularly exciting area of inquiry because the identification of specific genes for temperament will provide an important first step in understanding how genes influence temperament.

Conclusions

Nearly all temperament theories presume a biological basis to individual differences in early appearing, enduring behavioral tendencies considered to be temperamental. Consistent with these theories, most dimensions of temperament have demonstrated moderate genetic influences in twin and adoption studies. However, the finding of genetic influences on temperament is only a first step in the understanding of individual differences in early personality development. As illustrated in this paper, behavioral genetic methods can address a wide array of issues of relevance to child development. Researchers have begun to document the importance of environmental factors on temperament dimensions, track the developmental course of genetic and environmental contributions to temperament, address the issue of genetic and environmental overlap between temperament and problem behaviors, and relate specific genetic markers to temperament dimensions. These are just a sampling of the contributions that behavioral genetics has made to the understanding of temperament. Other research has looked at genetic and environmental contributions to the links between temperament and parenting103, sibling104 or peer relationships105, measure-specific and situational effects on temperament106, and genotype-environment correlations between temperament and the child’s environment107 – just to name a few. It is predicted that behavioral genetics research will make even greater contributions to our understanding of temperament as researchers continue to move beyond the basic heritability question.

Table 1

Individual differences in temperament can be due to:DefinitionExamplesIf important to the behavior under study:Temperament Findings
1. Genes. Segregating genes (i.e., genes that differ in a population). Specific genes that have been associated with behaviors. - result in behavioral similarities between genetically related individuals. Account for approximately 20 to 60% of the variability in most temperament dimensions.
2. Shared environments. Also known as “common” environments or “between family” environmental variance. Environmental influences that are shared by family members. Neighborhoods; schools1, parent education, family socioeconomic status, family religion, family vacations, pets, and household facilities (e.g., number of books in the home, swimming pool, piano, etc.) - result in behavioral similarities between family members. Little evidence of any shared environmental influences on temperament. Possible exceptions are positive affect, rhythmicity, soothability, shyness, and activity level.
3. Nonshared Environments. Also known as “unique” environments or “within family” environmental variance. Environmental influences that are specific to an individual. Friends2, peers2, teachers2, birth order, differential parental treatment, accidents, illnesses, and (random) measurement error. - result in behavioral differences between family members. Environmental influences on temperament tend to be of the nonshared variety, accounting for the remaining 40 to 80% of the variance that is not explained by genes.

Footnotes

This work was supported in part by grant MH062375 from the National Institute of Mental Health.

References

1. Goldsmith HH, Buss AH, Plomin R, et al. Roundtable: What is temperament? Four approaches. Child Dev. 1987;58:505–529. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

2. Plomin, R., Chipuer, H. M., & Neiderhiser, J. M. (1994). Behavioral genetic evidence for the importance of nonshared environment. In E. M. Hetherington, D. Reiss, & R. Plomin (Eds.), Separate social worlds of siblings: Importance of nonshared environment on development. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

3. Plomin, R., DeFries, J. C., McClearn, G. E., et al., (2001). Behavioral Genetics. (4th ed). Worth Publishing, NY.

4. Thomas, A. & Chess, S. (1977). Temperament and Development. New York, NY: Bruner/Mazel.

5. Rowe DC, Plomin R. Temperament in early childhood. J Pers Assess. 1977;41:150–156. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

6. Bayley, N. (1969). Manual for the Bayley Scales of Infant Development. New York: The Psychological Corporation.

7. Buss, A. H., & Plomin, R. (1984). Temperament: Early developing personality traits Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

8. Matheny AP., Jr Bayley’s Infant Behavior Record: Behavioral components and twin analysis. Child Dev. 1980;51:1157–1167. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

9. Matheny AP., Jr A longitudinal twin study of stability of components from Bayley’s Infant Behavior Record. Child Dev. 1983;54:356–360. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

10. Cyphers LH, Phillips K, Fulker DW, et al. Twin temperament during the transition from infancy to early childhood. J Am Acad Child Adoles Psychiatry. 1990;29:392–397. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

11. Goldsmith HH, Buss KA, Lemery KS. Toddler and childhood temperament: Expanded content, stronger genetic evidence, new evidence for the importance of environment. Dev Psychol. 1997;33:891–905. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

12. Saudino, K. J., & Cherny, S. S. (2001). Parent ratings of temperament in twins. In R. N. Emde & J. K. Hewitt (Eds.) The transition from infancy to early childhood: Genetic and environmental influences in the MacArthur Longitudinal Twin Study (pp.73–88). New York: Oxford University Press.

13. Stevenson J, Fielding J. Ratings of temperament in families of young twins. Br J Dev Psychol. 1985;3:143–152. [Google Scholar]

14. Neale MC, Stevenson J. Rater bias in the EASI temperament scales: A twin study. J Per Soc Psychol. 1989;56:446–455. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

15. Plomin R, Emde R, Braungart JM, et al. Genetic change and continuity from 14 to 20 months: The MacArthur Longitudinal Twin Study. Child Dev. 1993;64:1354–1376. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

16. Plomin, R., & DeFries, J. C. (1985). Origins of individual differences in infancy: The Colorado Adoption Project Toronto: Academic Press.

17. Plomin, R., DeFries, J. C., & Fulker, D. W. (1988). Nature and nurture during infancy and early childhood. New York: Cambridge University Press.

18. Gagne, J. R., Saudino, K. J. & Cherny, S. S. (2003). Genetic influences on temperament in early adolescence: A multimethod perspective. In S. Petrill, R. Plomin, J.C. DeFries & J. K. Hewitt (Eds.). The Transition to Early Adolescence: Nature and Nurture (pp. 166–184). New York: Oxford University Press.

19. Plomin R, Coon H, Carey G, et al. Parent-offspring and sibling adoption analyses of parental ratings of temperament in infancy and early childhood. J Per. 1991;59:705–732. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

20. Schmitz, S. (1994). Personality and temperament. In J. C. DeFries, R. Plomin, & D. W. Fulker (Eds.), Nature and nurture during middle childhood (pp. 120–140). Cambridge, MA: Blackwell.

21. Saudino KJ, McGuire S, Reiss D, et al. Parent ratings of EAS temperaments in twins, full siblings, half siblings and step siblings. J Per Soc Psychol. 1995;68:723–733. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

22. Cherny SS, Fulker DW, Corley JC, et al. Continuity and change in infant shyness from 14 to 20 months. Behav Genet. 1994;24:365–380. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

23. Matheny AP., Jr A longitudinal twin study of stability of components from Bayley’s Infant Behavior Record. Child Dev. 1983;54:356–360. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

24. Robinson JL, Kagan J, Reznick JS, et al. The heritability of inhibited and uninhibited behavior: A twin study. Dev Psychol. 1992;28:1030–1037. [Google Scholar]

25. Saudino, K. J., & Cherny, S. S. (2001a). Sources of continuity and change in observed temperament. In R. N. Emde & J. K. Hewitt (Eds.) The transition from infancy to early childhood: Genetic and environmental influences in the MacArthur Longitudinal Twin Study (pp. 89–110). New York: Oxford University Press.

26. Saudino KJ, Eaton WO. Infant temperament and genetics: An objective twin study of motor activity level. Child Dev. 1991;62:1167–1174. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

27. Saudino KJ, Eaton WO. Continuity and change in objectively assessed temperament: A longitudinal twin study of activity level. Br J Dev Psychol. 1995;13:81–95. [Google Scholar]

28. Emde, R. N. & Hewitt, J. K. (2001). The transition from infancy to early childhood: Genetic and environmental influences in the MacArthur Longitudinal Twin Study. New York: Oxford University Press.

29. Saudino KJ, Cherny SS, Plomin R. Parent Ratings of Temperament in Twins: Explaining the Atoo low@ DZ correlations. Twin Res. 2000;3:224–233. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

30. Saudino, K. J., Thompson, L. A., & Gagne, J. R. (June, 2000). Genetic influences on activity level in adolescence: Do the same genetic effects operate on parent ratings, self ratings and mechanical measures? Paper presented at the 30th Annual Meeting of the Behavior Genetics Association, Burlington, VT.

31. Schmitz S, Saudino KJ, Plomin R, et al. Genetic and environmental influences on temperament in middle childhood: Analyses of teacher and tester ratings. Child Dev. 1996;67:409–422. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

32. Braungart JM, Plomin R, DeFries JC, et al. Genetic influence on tester-rated infant temperament as assessed by Bayley’s Infant Behavior Record: Nonadoptive and adoptive siblings and twins. Dev Psychol. 1992;28:40–47. [Google Scholar]

33. Saudino KJ, McGuire S, Reiss D, et al. Parent ratings of EAS temperaments in twins, full siblings, half siblings and step siblings. J Per Soc Psychol. 1995;68:723–733. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

34. Goldsmith HH, Hewitt EC. Validity of parental report of temperament: Distinctions and needed research. Infant Behav Dev. 2002;26:108–111. [Google Scholar]

35. Hwang J, Rothbart MK. Behavior genetics studies of infant temperament: Findings vary across parent-report instruments. Infant Behav Dev. 2002;2:112–114. [Google Scholar]

36. Plomin R. The difficult concept of temperament: A response to Thomas, Chess, and Korn. Merrill Palmer Quarterly. 1982;28:25–33. [Google Scholar]

37. Saudino, K. J., Wertz, A., Gagne, J. R., et al. (in press). Night and day: Are siblings as different in temperament as parents say they are? J Per Soc Psychol. [PMC free article] [PubMed]

38. Saudino KJ, Plomin R, DeFries JC. Tester-rated temperament at 14, 20, and 24 months: Environmental change and genetic continuity. Br J Dev Psychol. 1996;14:129–144. [Google Scholar]

39. Eriksson GS, Pehrsson G. Relationships between the family’s way of functioning and children’s temperament as rated by parents of pre-term children. J Child Health Care. 2003;7:89–100. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

40. Leve LD, Scaramella LV, Fagot BI. Infant temperament, pleasure in parenting, and marital happiness in adoptive families. Infant Mental Health Journal. 2001;22:545–558. [Google Scholar]

41. Cohen DJ, Dibble E, Grawe JM. Fathers’ and mothers’ perceptions of children’s personality. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1977;34:480–487. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

42. Goldsmith, H. H., & Campos, J. J. (1986). Fundamental issues in the study of early temperament: The Denver Twin Temperament Study. In M. E. Lamb, A. L. Brown, & B. Rogoff (Eds.) Advances in Developmental Psychology (Vol 4, pp. 231–283). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

43. Goldsmith HH, Gottesman II. Origins of variation in behavioral style: A longitudinal study of temperament in young twins. Child Dev. 1981;52:91–103. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

44. Goldsmith HH, Lemery KS, Buss KA, et al. Genetic analyses of focal aspects of infant temperament. Dev Psychol. 1999;35:972–985. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

45. Lytton, H. (1980). Parent-child interaction: The socialization process observed in twin and singleton families. New York: Plenum Press.

46. Plomin R, Foch TT. A twin study of objectively assessed personality in childhood. J Per Soc Psychol. 1980;39:680–688. [Google Scholar]

47. Plomin R, Nesselroade JR. Behavioral genetics and personality change. J Per. 1990;58:191–220. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

48. McCartney K, Harris MJ, Bernieri F. Growing up and growing apart: A developmental meta-analysis of twin studies. Psychol Bull. 1990;107:226–237. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

49. Riese ML. Neonatal temperament in monozygotic and dizygotic twin pairs. Child Dev. 1990;61:1230–1237. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

50. Matheny AP. Children’s behavioral inhibition over age and across situations: Genetic similarity for a trait during change. J Per. 1989;57:215–235. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

51. Wohlwill, J. F. (1973). The study of behavioral development New York: Academic Press.

52. Matheny AP., Jr Developmental research of twin’s temperament. Acta Genet Med Gemellol. 1987;36:135–143. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

53. Wilson, R. S., & Matheny, A. P., Jr. (1986). Behavior genetics research in infant temperament: The Louisville twin study. In R. Plomin, & J. Dunn (Eds.), The study of temperament: Changes, continuities and challenges, (pp. 81–97). Hillsdale, N. J.: Erlbaum.

54. Plomin R. Multivariate analysis and developmental behavioral genetics: Developmental change as well as continuity. Behav Genet. 1986;16:25–43. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

55. Saudino KJ, Plomin R, DeFries JC. Tester-rated temperament at 14, 20, and 24 months: Environmental change and genetic continuity. Br J Dev Psychol. 1996;14:129–144. [Google Scholar]

56. McGue M, Bacon S, Lykken DT. Personality stability and change in early adulthood: A behavioral genetic analysis. Dev Psychol. 1993;29:96–109. [Google Scholar]

57. Rutter M. Temperament, personality, and personality disorder. Br J Psychiatry. 1987;150:443–458. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

58. Campbell SB. Behavior problems in preschool children: A review of recent research. J Child Psychol. 1995;36:113–149. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

59. Lavigne JV, Gibbons RD, Christoffel KK, et al. Prevalence rates and correlates of psychiatric disorders among preschool children. J Am Acad Child Adoles Psychiatry. 1996;35:204–214. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

60. Fagan J. The interaction between child sex and temperament in predicting behavior problems of preschool-age children in day care. Early Child Dev and Care. 1990;59:1–9. [Google Scholar]

61. Guerin DW, Gottfried AW, Thomas CW. Difficult temperament and behavior problems: A longitudinal study from 1. 5 to 12 years. Int J Behav Dev. 1997;21:71–90. [Google Scholar]

62. Lengua LJ, West SG, Sandler IN. Temperament as a predictor of symptomatology in children: Addressing contamination of measures. Child Dev. 1988;69:164–181. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

63. Rubin KH, Burgess KB, Dwyer KM, et al. Predicting preschoolers’ externalizing behaviors from toddler temperament, conflict, and maternal negativity. Dev Psychol. 2003;39:164–176. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

64. Fagot BI, O’ Brien M. Activity level in young children: Cross-age stability, situational influences, correlates with temperament, and the perception of problem behaviors. Merrill Palmer Quarterly. 1994;40:378–398. [Google Scholar]

65. Mehregany DV. The relation of temperament and behavior disorders in a preschool clinical sample. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev. 1991;22:129–136. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

66. Teglasi H, MacMahon BH. Temperament and common problem behaviors of children. J Appl Dev Psychol. 1990;11:331–349. [Google Scholar]

67. Goldsmith HH, Lemery KS. Linking temperamental fearfulness and anxiety symptoms: A behavioral-genetic perspective. Biol Psychiatry. 2000;48:1199–1209. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

68. Schmitz S, Fulker DW, Plomin R, et al. Temperament and problem behavior during early childhood. Int J Behav Dev. 1999;23:333–355. [Google Scholar]

69. Lemery KS, Essex MJ, Smider NA. Revealing the relation between temperament and behavior problem symptoms by eliminating measurement confounding: Expert ratings and factor analyses. Child Dev. 2002;73:867–882. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

70. Edelbrock C, Rende R, Plomin R, et al. A twin study of competence and problem behavior in childhood and early adolescence. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1995;36:775–785. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

71. Leve LD, Winebarger AA, Fagot BI, et al. Environmental and genetic variance in children’s observed and reported maladaptive behavior. Child Dev. 1998;69:1286–1298. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

72. Saudino, K. J., Ronald, A., & Plomin, R. (in press). Etiology of Behavior Problems in 7-year-old Twins: Substantial Genetic Influence and Negligible Shared Environmental Influence for Parent Ratings and Ratings by Same and Different Teachers. J Abnorm Child Psychol. [PubMed]

73. van den Oord E, Verhulst FC, Boomsma DI. A genetic study of maternal and paternal ratings of problem behaviors in 3 year old twins. J Abnorm Psychol. 1996;105:349–357. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

74. van der Valk JC, Verhulst FC, Neale MC, et al. Longitudinal genetic analysis of problem behaviors in biologically related and unrelated adoptees. Behav Genet. 1998;28:365–380. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

75. Schmitz, S. & Saudino, K. J. (2003). Links between temperament and behavior problems in children. In S. Petrill, R. Plomin, J.C. DeFries & J. K. Hewitt (Eds.). The Transition to Early Adolescence: Nature and Nurture (pp. 185–198). New York: Oxford University Press.

76. Gjone H, Stevenson J. A longitudinal twin study of temperament and behavior problems: Common genetic or environmental influences? J Am Acad Child Adoles Psychiatry. 1997;36:1448–1456. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

77. Plomin R, Owen MJ, McGuffin P. The genetic basis of complex human behaviors. Science. 1994;264:1733–1739. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

78. Plomin, R., & Saudino, K. J. (1994). Quantitative Genetics and Molecular Genetics. In J. E. Bates & T. D. Wachs (Eds.), Temperament: Individual differences at the interface of biology and behavior (pp. 143–171). Washington, DC: APA Books.

79. Asherson P, Curran S. Approaches to gene mapping in complex disorders and their application in Child Psychiatry and Psychology. Br J Psychiatry. 2001;179:122–128. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

80. Mill J, Curran S, Kent L, et al. Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and the dopamine D4 receptor gene: evidence of association but no linkage in a UK sample. Mol Psychiatry. 2001;6:440–444. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

81. Campbell SB, Breaux AM. Maternal ratings of activity level and symptomatic behaviors in a nonclinical sample of young children. J Pediatr Psychol. 1983;8:73–82. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

82. Klein HA. Early childhood group care: Predicting adjustment from individual temperament. J Genetic Psychol. 1980;137:125–131. [Google Scholar]

83. O=Donnell JP, Van Tuinan M. Behavior problems of preschool children: Dimensions and congenital correlates. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 1979;7:61–75. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

84. Schaughency EC, Fagot BI. The prediction of adjustment at age 7 from activity level at age 5. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 1993;21:29–50. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

85. Hall SJ, Halperin JM, Schwartz ST, et al. Behavioral and executive functions in children with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder and reading disability. J Attent Disord. 1997;1:235–247. [Google Scholar]

86. Halperin JM, Newcorn JH, Matier K. Discriminant validity of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. J Am Acad Child Adoles Psychiatry. 1993;32:1038–1043. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

87. Matier-Sharma K, Perachio N, Newcorn JH, et al. Differential diagnosis of ADHD: Are objective measures of attention, impulsivity, and activity level helpful? Child Neuropsychol. 1995;1:118–127. [Google Scholar]

88. McIntosh DE, Cole-Love AS. Profile comparisons between ADHD and non-ADHD children on the Temperament Assessment Battery for Children. J Psychoeducational Assessment. 1996;14:362–372. [Google Scholar]

89. Reif A, Lesch K. Toward a molecular architecture of personality. Behav Brain Res. 2003;139:1–20. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

90. Benjamin J, Li L, Patterson C, et al. Population and familial association between the D4 dopamine receptor gene and measures of novelty seeking. Nat Genet. 1996;12:81–84. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

91. Ebstein RP, Novick O, Umansky R, et al. Dopamine D4 receptor (D4DR) exon III polymorphism associated with the human personality trait of novelty seeking. Nat Genet. 1996;12:78–80. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

92. Lesch KP, Bengel D, Heils A, et al. Association of anxiety-related traits with a polymorphism in the serotonin transporter gene regulatory region. Science. 1996;274:1527–1531. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

93. Flory JD, Manuck SB, Ferrell RE, et al. Neuroticism is not associated with the serotonin transporter (5-HTTLPR) polymorphism. Mol Psychiatry. 1999;4:93–96. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

94. Jonsson EG, Nothen MM, Gustavsson JP, et al. Lack of evidence for allelic association between personality traits and the dopamine D4 receptor gene polymorphisms. Am J Psychiatry. 1997;154:697–699. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

95. Pogue-Geile M, Ferrell R, Deka R, et al. Human novelty-seeking personality traits and Dopamine D4 receptor polymorphisms: A twin and genetic association study. Am J Med Genet. 1998;81:44–48. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

96. Auerbach J, Geller V, Lezer S, et al. Dopamine D4 receptor (D4DR) and serotonin transporter promoter (5-HTTLPR) polymorphisms in the determination of temperament in 2-month-old infants. Mol Psychiatry. 1999;4:369–373. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

97. Ebstein RP, Levine J, Geller V, et al. Dopamine D4 receptor and serotonin transporter promoter in the determination of neonatal temperament. Mol Psychiatry. 1998;3:238–246. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

98. Auerbach JG, Benjamin J, Faroy M. DRD4 related to infant attention and information processing: A developmental link to ADHD? Psychiatr Genet. 2001;11:31–35. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

99. Auerbach JG, Faroy M, Ebstein R. The association of the dopamine D4 receptor gene (DRD4) and the serotonin transporter promotor gene (5-HTTLPR) with temperament in 12-month-old infants. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2001;42:777–783. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

100. Lakatos K, Nemoda Z, Birkas E. Association of D4 dopamine receptor gene and serotonin transporter promoter polymorphisms with infants’ response to novelty. Mol Psychiatry. 2003;8:90–97. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

101. Schmidt LA, Fox NA, Rubin KH. Molecular genetics of shyness and aggression in preschoolers. Per & Indiv Diff. 2002;33:227–238. [Google Scholar]

102. De Luca A, Rizzardi M, Torrente I, et al. Dopamine D4 receptor (DRD4) polymorphism and adaptability trait during infancy : a longitudinal study in 1- to 5-month-old neonates. Neurogenets. 2001;3:79–82. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

103. Petrill SA, Deater-Deckard K. Task orientation, parental warmth and SES account for a significant proportion of the shared environmental variance in general cognitive ability in early childhood: Evidence from a twin study. Dev Sci. 2004;7:25–32. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

104. Lemery KS, Goldsmith HH. Genetic and environmental influences on preschool sibling cooperation and conflict: Associations with difficult temperament and parenting style. Marriage & Family Review. 2002;33:77–99. [Google Scholar]

105. Pike A, Atzaba-Poria N. Do sibling and friend relationships share the same temperamental origins? A twin study. J Child Psychol & Psychiatry. 2003;44:598–611. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

106. Philips K, Matheny AP. Evidence for genetic influence on both cross-situation and situation-specific components of behavior. J Per Soc Psychol. 1997;73:129–138. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

107. Saudino KJ, Plomin R. Cognitive and temperamental mediators of genetic contributions to the home environment during infancy. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly. 1997;43:1–23. [Google Scholar]

Which of the following statements is true of functionalism quizlet?

Which of the following statements is true of functionalism? It probes the purposes of the mind and behavior in an individual's adaptation to the environment.

Which method allows psychologists and other scientists to observe behavior where it happens or in the field?

Psychologists use the scientific method to observe behaviors as they occur in everyday life and in situations researchers are interested in learning about.

What is defined by heredity and is based on biological structures and processes?

Heredity. Defines one's nature--which is based on one's biological structures and processes. Refers to the biological transmission of traits that have evolved from generation to generation. Genetics. The area of biology that focuses on heredity.

What is a complete group of interest to researchers from which a sample is drawn for an experiment?

A “population of interest” is defined as the population/group from which a researcher tries to draw conclusions. It is a subset of the general population that the surveyor wants to know more about. Many research studies require specific groups of interest to make decisions based on their findings.